Cranberry word: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>David E. Volk
No edit summary
mNo edit summary
 
(4 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 2: Line 2:
''This article is about [[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]]. For the plant, see [[cranberry]].''
''This article is about [[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]]. For the plant, see [[cranberry]].''


A '''cranberry word''', in [[linguistics]] and [[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]], is an exceptional compound [[word]] that is stored in the [[lexicon]] (or 'mental dictionary') in its full form, rather than being built from productive rules of [[grammar]]. The typical example is ''cranberry'', which unlike ''teapot'', could not have been formed from a combination of two words, since *''cran-'' is not a word. Sometimes cranberry words are referred to as ''fossilised terms'' or ''cranberry [[morpheme]]s''.
A '''cranberry word''', in [[linguistics]] and [[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]], is an exceptional compound [[word]] that is stored in the [[lexicon]] (or 'mental dictionary') in its full form, rather than being built from productive rules of [[grammar]]. The typical example is ''cranberry'', which, unlike ''teapot'', could not have been formed from a combination of two words, since *''cran'' is not a word. Sometimes cranberry words are referred to as ''fossilised terms'' or ''cranberry [[morpheme]]s''.


[[Language]]s often form new words by combining existing words. Because speakers can always come up with new, acceptable words, this process appears to be a consequence of a rule within the minds of the users of the language. For example, in [[English language|English]], a rule allows for the formation of words such as ''schoolboy'' and ''teapot''. Of two words, one can be assigned ''headedness'' (i.e. is more important); in English, most compounds are ''endocentric'', or headed in this way. Speakers will judge that ''pot'' in ''teapot'' is the head; in English the head is usually rightmost. The head will take [[inflection]]s such as [[plurality|plural]] ''-s''. The remaining part of the compound (e.g. ''tea'') modifies the head, so in this case the compound describe a kind of pot, not a kind of [[tea]].<ref>Katamba & Stonham (2006: 316-336).</ref>
[[Language]]s often form new words by combining existing words. Because speakers can always come up with new, acceptable words, this process appears to be a consequence of a rule within the minds of the users of the language. For example, in [[English language|English]], a rule allows for the formation of words such as ''schoolboy'' and ''teapot''. Of two words, one can be assigned ''headedness'' (i.e. is more important); in English, most compounds are ''endocentric'', or headed in this way. Speakers will judge that ''pot'' in ''teapot'' is the head; in English the head is usually rightmost. The head will take [[inflection]]s such as [[plurality|plural]] ''-s''. The remaining part of the compound (e.g. ''tea'') modifies the head, so in this case the compound describes a kind of pot, not a kind of [[tea]].<ref>Katamba & Stonham (2006: 316-336).</ref>


This procedure is 'productive', meaning that speakers can use the rule to create new words. With cranberry words, this procedure is blocked. Although such words can be analysed as two units, one is either not a word in its own right, or means something different. Therefore, they could not have been formed from kind of rules set out above. A more likely interpretation is that they stand alone as [[lexis|lexical]] exceptions.<ref>Anderson (1990).</ref>
This procedure is 'productive', meaning that speakers can use the rule to create new words. With cranberry words, this procedure is blocked. Although such words can be analysed as two units, one is either not a word in its own right, or means something different. Therefore, they could not have been formed from kind of rules set out above. A more likely interpretation is that they stand alone as [[lexis|lexical]] exceptions.<ref>Anderson (1990).</ref>


Some words appear to be derived from a productive rule, but cannot be divided into two units that are indisputably related to the compound. For example, ''blackberry'' appears to be formed from the [[adjective]] ''black'' and the [[noun]] ''berry'', yet not all berries that are black are [[blackberry|blackberries]]. Looking at other [[berry|berries]], the word ''gooseberry'' seems involve a word that in isolation most often refers to a bird. Since the ''goose-'' of ''gooseberry'' is not the same goose as an avian [[goose]], there is no justification for treating them as the same word stored in the [[mind]]'s lexicon; more likely, ''gooseberry'' is not a compound, but a word in its own right.<ref>Katamba & Stonham (2006: 334-336).</ref>
Some words appear to be derived from a productive rule, but cannot be divided into two units that are indisputably related to the compound. For example, ''blackberry'' appears to be formed from the [[adjective]] ''black'' and the [[noun]] ''berry'', yet not all berries that are black are [[blackberry|blackberries]]. Looking at other [[berry|berries]], the word ''gooseberry'' seems involve a word that in isolation most often refers to a bird. Since the ''goose-'' of ''gooseberry'' is not the same goose as an avian [[goose]], there is no justification for treating them as the same word stored in the [[mind]]'s lexicon; more likely, ''gooseberry'' is not a compound, but a word in its own right.<ref>Katamba & Stonham (2006: 334-336).</ref>
==Footnotes==
<div class="references-2column">
<references/>
</div>


==References==
==References==
Line 27: Line 22:
*[[Syntax]]
*[[Syntax]]


[[Category: CZ Live]]
==Footnotes==
{{reflist}}[[Category:Suggestion Bot Tag]]

Latest revision as of 16:00, 2 August 2024

This article is developing and not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
 
This editable Main Article is under development and subject to a disclaimer.

This article is about morphology. For the plant, see cranberry.

A cranberry word, in linguistics and morphology, is an exceptional compound word that is stored in the lexicon (or 'mental dictionary') in its full form, rather than being built from productive rules of grammar. The typical example is cranberry, which, unlike teapot, could not have been formed from a combination of two words, since *cran is not a word. Sometimes cranberry words are referred to as fossilised terms or cranberry morphemes.

Languages often form new words by combining existing words. Because speakers can always come up with new, acceptable words, this process appears to be a consequence of a rule within the minds of the users of the language. For example, in English, a rule allows for the formation of words such as schoolboy and teapot. Of two words, one can be assigned headedness (i.e. is more important); in English, most compounds are endocentric, or headed in this way. Speakers will judge that pot in teapot is the head; in English the head is usually rightmost. The head will take inflections such as plural -s. The remaining part of the compound (e.g. tea) modifies the head, so in this case the compound describes a kind of pot, not a kind of tea.[1]

This procedure is 'productive', meaning that speakers can use the rule to create new words. With cranberry words, this procedure is blocked. Although such words can be analysed as two units, one is either not a word in its own right, or means something different. Therefore, they could not have been formed from kind of rules set out above. A more likely interpretation is that they stand alone as lexical exceptions.[2]

Some words appear to be derived from a productive rule, but cannot be divided into two units that are indisputably related to the compound. For example, blackberry appears to be formed from the adjective black and the noun berry, yet not all berries that are black are blackberries. Looking at other berries, the word gooseberry seems involve a word that in isolation most often refers to a bird. Since the goose- of gooseberry is not the same goose as an avian goose, there is no justification for treating them as the same word stored in the mind's lexicon; more likely, gooseberry is not a compound, but a word in its own right.[3]

References

  • Anderson SR (1990) 'A-morphous morphology'. Manuscript, Cognitive Science Center. Baltimore: John Hopkins University.
  • Katamba F & Stonham J (2006) Morphology. Basingstoke: Palgrave. 2nd edition. ISBN 1403916440.

See also

Footnotes

  1. Katamba & Stonham (2006: 316-336).
  2. Anderson (1990).
  3. Katamba & Stonham (2006: 334-336).